 |
Mental body, a
clinical
expression of
the mind
Summary:
The
author presents
the "mental
body" as an
alternative
hypothesis for
the approach of
the mind. At
present the mind
is seen as a
particular set
of functions
performed by the
brain. This
model seems to
give the mind a
notion
compatible with
the organism as
a whole.
|
|
|
Using the method
as neurological
examination, we
demonstrate the
existence of a
"mental body",
which is shown
in many clinical
situations such
as in hysteria,
hypnosis, in
narcolepsy, the
phantom limb and
called out of
body
experiences.
This way of
studying the
mind from the
perspective of a
body that
identifies
semiology can
open a vast
field of
experimentation,
and
interpretation
of both
psychological
and neurological
phenomena.
Introduction
The math tells
us that the
elements of a
set cannot
explain the
entire nature of
this set. The
concept of the
whole escapes
that each party
alone can
represent (1.2
Bertrand
Russell).
Considering the
brain neurons as
elements of a
set which is
assumed to
contain the
mind, we
question whether
it is possible a
thorough
understanding of
the concept of
mind based on
the functions of
neurons. This
question allows
us, at least
theoretically,
put the mind as
lying, both
outside and
within the set
of brain
neurons.
Moreover, new
theories (Ilya
Prigogine in Del
Nero 3), suggest
that "systems of
high complexity"
have the
capacity to
organize
themselves. The
nervous system
in addition to
their physical
structure, can
be seen as a
highly complex
biosystem,
endowed with
specific
properties and
peculiarities of
living beings. A
"theory of mind"
in April, taken
as monistic,
materialistic
and "emerging",
identifies the
"mental states"
as a distinct
subset of the
"brain states"
that are clearly
of a physical
nature, and that
would, in turn,
a subset of
states of the
nervous system.
According to
this theory, the
activities of
neurons in your
trading
electrochemical
produce a new
quality of
phenomena
"emerge" as a
mental function,
similar to the
order which
results in
systems of high
complexity.
The various
theories of mind
four currently
available
cannot, however,
with good
chances of
passing
theoretic
framework,
without which
they can realize
a whole range of
phenomena known
that mental
activity
expresses. No
theory has yet
managed to make
specific
predictions
about the mental
phenomena, much
less assured us
the opportunity
to test it in
clinical or
laboratory.
Objective and
method
It is precisely
the possibility
of testing the
hypothesis both
in terms of
clinical and
laboratory, I am
suggesting the
concept of
"mental body"
instead of the
mind. I present
several
situations where
the neurological
examination can
confirm this
hypothesis as
consistent with
the clinical
expressions. In
this work,
consider the
mental body as a
model that has
an identity
clinic, which
can be revealed
by assessment
tools that
offers a
neurological
examination.
Semiological
models
Hysteria
- Patients who
present
hysterical
sensory or motor
disorders show a
pattern typical
semiological,
noting, first of
all, they do not
obey the
anatomical
distributions
appropriate to
the various
pathways of
innervation of
the nervous
system.
On the other
hand, in organic
brain lesions,
the map shows
distributions of
anesthesia very
familiar to
neurologists,
who have learned
to see the
levels of
anesthesia
metameric (1) or
halomeric and
syndromes called
alternate,
characterized by
impairment of an
anesthetic in
hemifacial side
and the trunk
and limbs on the
contralateral
hemisphere.
Semiological
studies show
that the
hysterical
patient is a
different
pattern of
anesthesia,
compromising,
sometimes your
whole body, he
does not know
that the sensory
innervation of
the face runs
through the
trigeminal
nerve, while the
posterior
regions of the
scalp, neck,
follow
innervations
very distant, at
the level of
cervical spinal
cord. Anesthesia
in members of
the hysterical
spare no way
sensitivity of
impairment of
global
sensitivities
and shallow. The
organization of
this "anatomy"
produced by the
hysteric is the
product of
mental
conception than
it does in your
body. The
hysteric
symptomatology
is expressed as
having a "body"
organized by his
mind and not his
brain. This
attitude is
known in the
history of
hysteria and,
undoubtedly, is
universal, as
can be read in
one of the
classics of
neurology, "sémiologie
des Affections
du système
nerveux" J.
Dejerine (1914),
5. In the
symptomatic
evaluation of
the hysteric can
identify how he
expresses his
mental body.
Hysterical
paralysis also
reveals
contrasts with
the semiology of
lesional organic
syndromes. The
sagging is
extravagant,
hypertonia
usually diffuse
throughout the
muscle, not
respecting the
distribution
between agonists
(2) and
antagonists that
the end system
demands. The leg
of this patient
will offer much
resistance to
being bent as to
be extended. The
hemiplegic or
paraplegic
hysterical
builds a
disability
within an
imaginary model,
under a mental
construct and
not a loss of
nerve pathways.
Hypnosis
- individuals
who assimilate
the suggestions
that induce
hypnosis can
produce both
anesthesia and
paralysis. The
medical
experience, this
vast área6 has
demonstrated
that the
paralysis and
anesthesia
follow the same
pattern of
hysterical
frames (7, 8,
9). In either
picture, we
realize that the
"body" built by
the hysterical
and is
mesmerized by
the rise in
their "mental
models" and does
not obey the
systematization
of neural
pathways.
The memories of
the hypnotized
- The common
experience of
the hypnotic
trance we know
that upon
awakening, the
hypnotized does
not retain the
memories of what
he heard or
played during
the trance. A
second induction
made soon after
the rescue makes
these memories
returning to the
scene of the
first trance,
without
realizing now
what he heard or
did in the
interval between
two trances.
This experience
seems to reveal
two distinct
files of
memorization. I
would say that
one of them is
located in the
physical brain,
when he is
awake, and
others in the
mental body when
he is in a
trance. This
situation can be
compared to what
we do on the
computer: we
create a file
for certain text
does not open
the text of
another. For
that to happen,
you need to copy
and paste each
other to carry
out this
reading. In the
case of
hypnosis, we can
use hypnotic
suggestion to
transfer
memories from
one environment
to another,
which is
achieved with
relative ease.
Narcolepsy
- Narcolepsy is
a sleep disorder
in which the
patient suddenly
goes into a
state of
drowsiness he
cannot control.
The episodes are
repeated often
uncomfortably
disturbing the
patient's daily
activities. The
episodes often
varied and may
be a few minutes
or hours. On
awakening, these
patients are
reported
curious.
Apparently can
remain lucid
during
drowsiness,
performing
complex
activities
during this
period. They
feel they leave
the physical
body and live
with different
scenarios and
characters. Some
report a
timeless
experience, may
be witnesses of
past episodes or
that will be
confirmed in the
future. Anyway,
they seem to be
in possession of
a body with
which they
experience their
experiences. The
classics of
neurology label
these frames
hypnagogic
hallucinations.
Also included
here would be
called lucid
dreams that
normal subjects
report. It
seems, however,
that in
narcolepsy
experience is
more conscious
and less
symbolic than
the experiences
we all dream of.
It is not
difficult for
these patients
describe the
physical and
mental
functional body
that allows them
to pass through
their "dreams."
Phantom limb
- amputations
often occur in
violent
accidents can
have on patient
perceptions of
continuity of
existence of his
amputated limb
(amputation of
other body parts
such as breast,
nose, tongue,
penis and
scrotum can
produce symptoms
similar to
phantom limb) 10
. Melzack 11,
12, believes in
the existence in
the brain, an
image of the
entire body in a
neural array. It
would consist of
a network of
neural
interconnections,
organized and
genetically from
sensory stimuli,
creating a
pattern of
identification
that I Melzack
10 calls "neuro
signature. Even
children born
without limbs
can reveal the
existence of
such a matrix
body 11.
Notwithstanding
the
neurophysiological
hypotheses that
attempt to
explain the
symptoms of
phantom limb,
their clinical
manifestation
can complement
the examples of
the mental body
we want to
study. The
phantom limb
gives the
patient the
whole feel of a
real member (réalité
concrète du
sentiment,
according
Lhermitte) 10,
where he feels
pain, tickling,
spontaneous
movements and
reactions of
avoidance (3)
how to tap into
a mobile.
Considering that
member as part
of the mental
body, we see
that the
consciousness of
the patient does
not exercise
control over its
functions,
whether motor or
sensory. We can
say that this
lack of control
is relevant to
the tables of
hysteria and
hypnosis that
noted.
A number of
other clinical
phenomena seem
to suggest that
representation
of the embodied
mind that we are
analyzing. The
construction of
body image and
the syndromes of
neglect are good
examples. The
lay and
neuropsychiatric
literature
produced for
some time on a
multitude of
texts referring
to out of body
experiences and
near-death
experiences. We
neurologists
often encounter
between the
mental symptoms
of epilepsy, the
"notion of a
presence," where
an "entity"
appears to
follow the
proceedings as a
witness of the
seizure.
Comments
We have no doubt
that the dilemma
brain / mind is
inexhaustible,
sometimes
contradictory
and
irreconcilable.
Propose to
discuss the
issue in terms
of the mental
body, we know
the difficulty
of introducing a
new idea in the
context of such
complexity.
Remember,
however, a
statement of
evolutionist
Stephen Jay
Gould proposed
13 that the
evolution of the
species off.
"New facts,
collected the
old way, under
the tutelage of
old theories,
they rarely lead
to any
substantial
revision of
thought. The
facts do not
"speak for
themselves" are
read in light of
the theory.
Creative
thinking, both
science and the
arts, is the
engine for
change of
opinion."
The discussion
of the mind seem
to run out
between
Philosophy and
Science without
coming to an
end. The "mental
body" seems to
me that has the
merit of
specifying a
more appropriate
object of study
because their
clinical
behavior and
experimental.
We hope that
subsequent
studies can
prove the
validity of our
proposal. We
still need to
explore the
clinical
features of the
mental body and
identify their
anatomical and
functional
characteristics
crucial since it
can be
clinically
evaluated in
hysteria, tested
experimentally
in hypnosis,
recognized in
the phantom
limb, confirmed
in narcolepsy
and out of body
experiences, as
exemplify.
Nubor Facure
Orlando,
Campinas-SP, is
a former
professor of
neurosurgery at
UNICAMP and
director of the
Brain Institute.
Notes:
(1) comes from
metameric
metamerism, ie
division into
shares or
similar
segments,
arranged in a
linear series
along the
longitudinal
axis.
(2) agonist, in
anatomy, it is
said that
promotes muscle
action which is
contrasted with
another muscle,
called the
antagonist.
(3) Avoidance is
the same as
avoidance:
avoidance.
References:
1 - B. Russell
History of
Western thought:
the adventure of
the Presocratics
to Wittgenstein.
Rio de Janeiro -
Ediouro 2001
2 - Macrone M.
Eureka! A book
about ideas -
Sao Paulo - Ed
Rotterdan, 1997,
pp. 121 and 122.
3 - Del Nero H.S.
The site of the
mind: thought,
emotion and
desire in the
human brain. São
Paulo: Collegium
Cognitio, 1977,
Page 193.
4 - The
Tripicchio,
Tripicchio AC.
Theories of mind
- Ribeirão Preto,
SP, Ed Tecmedd,
2003, Page 72-77
5 - Dejerine J.
Sémiologie des
Affections du
Systeme nerveux
- 12 ed - Masson
et Cie Éditeurs,
Paris, 1914, pp.
540-549 and 927.
6 - Ferreira MV.
Hypnosis in
clinical
practice, São
Paulo, Ed
Atheneu, 2003
7 - Halligan PW,
Athwal, BS,
Oakley DA,
Franckowiak, RSJ.
Imaging Hipnotic
paralysis:
Implications for
conversion
hysteria. The
Lancet 2000,
355:986-987
8 - Halligan PW.
New approaches
to conversion
hysteria. BMJ
2000; 320:
1488-1489
(3june)
9 - Marshall JC,
Halligan PW,
Fink GR, Wade
DT, Frackwdak,
RSJ. The
functional
anatomy of a
hysterical
paralysis.
Cognition 1997,
64 (1), p. B1B8
10 - Jensen TS,
Rasmussen P.
Amputation. Pag
402-412.
Textbook of
pain, Ed Patrick
D. Wall, Ronald
Melzack
(Churchill
Livingstone),
London, 1984
11 - Melzack R,
Israel R,
Lacroix R,
Schultz G.
Phantom limbs in
people with
congenital limb
deficiency or
amputation in
early childhood.
Brain, 1997, 120
(9) 1603-1620
12 - Melzack R.
Phantom limbs.
Sci Am April
1992, 266:
120-126
13 - S. Gould J.
Darwin and the
great riddles of
life. Translated
by Mary
Elizabeth
Martinez, 2nd Ed
São Paulo -
Martins Fontes,
1999, Page 158.
|